Saturday, January 16, 2010

Anecdotal Schmanecdotal: Why We Need Less "Folk," and More Remedy

It would not be wise to rely on remedies that have only anecdotal evidence to support the claims made about them. Anecdotal evidence does have a role to play as a launching point for deeper inquiry. However, such evidence is subject to a host of logical fallacies that undermine its usefulness as a guide, especially for something as vital as medicine. Although there are limtations to the Food and Drug Administration’s (FDA) approval processes for medicines and drugs, the benefits of requiring clinical trials and quantifiable evidence to gain institutional legitimacy far outweigh any weaknesses inherent in the governing body that reviews such claims.

Anecdotal (sometimes called “testimonial”) evidence is unreliable for a multitude of reasons. Professor of Philosophy Robert T. Carroll, Ph.D., gives a long list of these reasons on the online counterpart to his 2003 book, The Skeptic’s Dictionary:



Stories are prone to contamination by beliefs, later experiences, feedback, selective attention to details, and so on. Most stories get distorted in the telling and the retelling. Events get exaggerated. Time sequences get confused. Details get muddled. Memories are imperfect and selective; they are often filled in after the fact. People misinterpret their experiences. Experiences are conditioned by biases, memories, and beliefs, so people's perceptions might not be accurate. Most people aren't expecting to be deceived, so they may not be aware of deceptions that others might engage in. Some people make up stories. Some stories are delusions. Sometimes events are inappropriately deemed psychic simply because they seem improbable when they might not be that improbable after all. In short, anecdotes are inherently problematic and are usually impossible to test for accuracy.


Personal experience may seem like the most reliable of all tests, but people are more easily fooled than they would like to believe – ask any magician. More rigorous investigation has shown again and again that many so-called “folk remedies,” some of which have been in use for centuries, simply are not efficacious beyond the psychological phenomenon known as the placebo effect.

Wheatgrass juice is a case in point. According to the Natural Medicines Comprehensive Database, people today drink this concoction as a curative for a wide range of ailments, including cancer, preventing gray hair, kidney stones, bronchitis, arthritis, and to rid the body of “toxins” (a catch-all term that means nothing from a medical standpoint).

In an article for the August 2008 issue of Scientific American magazine titled “Wheatgrass Juice and Folk Medicine,” Dr. Michael Shermer, Adjunct Professor of Economics at Claremont Graduate University and founder of Skeptic magazine, traced the modern promotion of wheatgrass to a holistic health practitioner named Ann Wigmore, who began campaigning for the plant in the 1940’s after being inspired by a biblical tale of King Nebuchadnezzer, as well as the tendency of dogs and cats to eat grass to aid in digestion.

According to Shermer, Wigmore posited that wheatgrass had such palliative powers because of the enzymes and chlorophylls contained therein. Wigmore, however, completely misunderstood how digestion works. To explain why it makes no sense, Shermer quoted William T. Jarvis, retired professor of public health at the Loma Linda University School of Medicine and founder of the National Council against Health Fraud:



“Enzymes are complex protein molecules produced by living organisms exclusively for their own use in promoting chemical reactions. Orally ingested enzymes are digested in the stomach and have no enzymatic activity in the eater.” Jarvis adds, “The fact that grass-eating animals are not spared from cancer, despite their large intake of fresh chlorophyll, seems to have been lost on Wigmore. In fact, chlorophyll cannot ‘detoxify the body’ because it is not absorbed.”


Ann Wigmore. Courtesy of healthbanquet.com

Despite the obvious inanity of Wigmore’s reasoning, there continues to be a vibrant market for wheatgrass, perpetuated entirely by personal testimony and careful marketing.

So what is the big deal? If products like wheatgrass juice - or traditional therapies like acupuncture or homeopathy - make someone feel better, why should it matter if there are no well-designed clinical trials to support any significant effects? Why does an organization like the FDA refuse to let these remedies join the ranks of the approved? What is the harm?

The harm comes in three forms. The first is that anecdotal claims often rest on unstated (and sometimes stated) premises that fly in the face of basic scientific principles. For instance: 160 years ago, Samuel Hahnemann, the father of homeopathy, based his methodology on the concept that water has “memory,” and that diluting a substance renders it more powerful. The most “effective” homeopathic remedies are usually so diluted that they do not contain a single molecule of the original substance. People who accept these ideas not only become misinformed about medicine – they must reject most of physics, chemistry, and biology to maintain their belief. The diametric opposition to well-established facts becomes a huge barrier to understanding many other aspects of science, and undermines critical thinking abilities altogether.

The second harm is borne by those with ills that might be treated by standard methods of care, but whose conditions are worsened because they choose alternatives that have only anecdotal backing. The conflation of correlation with causation is a common culprit in such anecdotal misinterpretations. If, for instance, person X receives homeopathic treatment for prostate cancer and discovers that the cancer has gone into remission, he may decide that the treatment caused the remission. In doing so, he ignores the fact that cancers sometimes spontaneously subside, that there may be other factors that contributed to his newfound health (exercise, other treatments, and so on), and – most importantly – that persons A to W did not obtain the same results. While X champions his homeopathic cure, these other folk never get the chance to tell the stories of their failures, because they are dead.

This is more than just a hypothetical scenario. Homeopaths have claimed that their remedies can effectively treat diseases like malaria, gonorrhea, and even AIDS. One such practitioner named Peter Chappell peddles his cures (which are proprietary, because, he says, they are “ahead of the science and because the terms are not there, to explain it within science”) to the sick and desperate in Africa. Chappell does some fancy footwork to explain his method’s ineffectiveness in the developed world. In a 2007 interview for the online homeopathic forum Hpathy, he said the following of “White people and Europeans”:


“White people with AIDS frequently don't respond so well to PC AIDS [the name of his remedy] because they have a lot of other issues running at the same time, which are more important often than the AIDS, in that they depress the immune system far more than the virus.”


Peter Chappell. Courtesy of minimum.com

Chappell seems to forget that Africans suffer from much higher rates of most diseases than anyone else on the planet, that they have poorer nutritional health, that they frequently face stressful and unsanitary living conditions, and that some African countries have average life spans that are about half as long as those enjoyed in rich countries. Yet white people (whatever that means), we are told, have more issues depressing their immune systems. Is it at all more likely that “white people” would require clinical trials, while desperate African nations are more willing to accept the word of anyone who promises hope for their ailing populations? As it stands, it is impossible to tell, since Chappell has no statistical evidence to back his claims.

Most people who rely on anecdotally supported treatments are not on the verge of death, of course, and many folk remedies are applied only in relatively benign situations, such as the occasional scrape or cold. Yet even in these cases, the consumer of folk remedies faces a third potential harm: a loss of money and time. According to a December 2009 article in the Nutrition Business Journal, which describes itself as “a research, publishing and consulting company serving the nutrition, natural products and alternative health care industries,” U.S. consumer sales for the subcategory of supplements that includes homeopathic remedies, fish/animal oils, probiotics, and a few other supplements grew by $270 million in 2008 to $4.5 billion. This, according to the article, was a “less than stellar” year for the products. Such huge investments by the American public divert funding from therapies and treatments that have more than testimony to vouch for them.

Anecdotal evidence is clearly not enough to make a folk remedy worth relying upon. This does not mean that folk remedies carry absolutely no weight, however. Just as they steer people in wrong directions, they have been the source of a great number of legitimate discoveries. One famous example of just such a remedy involved British sailors in the 1700’s who suffered from scurvy, a condition caused by Vitamin C deficiency. Dr. James Lind conducted one the earliest clinical trials in history, and discovered that sailors who were given a regimen of two oranges and one lime a day improved. In time, lime juice was substituted for lemon juice, and the sailors who imbibed this everyday substance were dubbed “limeys.”

James Lind. Courtesy of jameslindlibrary.org

More recently, certain fields that were once the realm of the humanities have begun to standardize their practices by taking more evidence-based approaches to research. This has been especially true of clinical psychology, which has coupled itself with neurobiology and other “hard sciences” as well as evaluating its internal methods to establish greater standards of efficacy and accountability. Discussions of creating a new accreditation system have spawned fresh debates over just how much the field can benefit. In a December 4 segment on National Public Radio’s Science Friday titled “Can Science Make Psychotherapy More Effective,” Dianne Chambless, the Merriam term professor of Psychology at the University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia, related how incorporating new evidence into her practice had improved outcomes in people with obsessive compulsive disorder:


…back in the 60s, early 70s, we were treating obsessive compulsive disorder with relaxation approaches because that seemed to help other anxiety disorders. And we weren't really getting anywhere.

And then in England people developed a new treatment called exposure and ritual prevention. And we read about this. I was in training. And we started with great excitement to try this new treatment, and it worked. So we weren't different as therapists. We were still the same caring people that we used to be. But we tried a new treatment that was based on the science of fear and learning. It worked. It meant some people who were crippled by their problems became functional people, and around 70 percent of them got very much better.


Chambless went on to say that despite the obvious difference between the two approaches, surveys of clinical psychologists have shown that more continue to use the defunct relaxation techniques. Both types of therapy became widespread because of anecdotal evidence. Further studies showed one to be more effective than the other. This, she believes, is just one reason to bring psychotherapeutic methods further beyond the realm of anecdote.

The problems with relying on remedies that are not supported by scientific evidence are multitudinous. This makes the need for an approval process for medicines obvious. However, no system is perfect. As the primary body for determining the safety and efficacy of medicines in the U.S., the FDA bears the weight of criticisms, some of which are legitimate, from all sides. As with any governmental organization, politics and money play a role in its function. Moreover, the very nature of the clinical trial may impede many remedies from being approved – or even reviewed.

The Mayo Clinic’s website offers a few reasons why some alternative medicines never make the cut for approval by the FDA. Among other things, it points out that “large, carefully controlled medical studies are costly. Trials for conventional medications or procedures are often directly or indirectly funded by the government or drug companies, giving conventional treatments more resources to do studies.”

When there are no lucrative patents to be gained, for-profit companies are less likely to pursue research into folk medicines. This lack of funding has historically kept many potentially beneficial remedies from being investigated.

As the alternative medicine industry has gained ground in popular culture, however, the government has taken an active role in funding studies through the branch of the National Institutes of Health called the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine (NCCAM). In 2008, approximately $300 million (around 1%) of funding for medical research went to this institution.

Graphic Courtesy of nccam.nih.gov

Clinical trials take years to conduct, and this poses another barrier to alternative modalities. Many of them make so many claims that investigating all of them would take decades. Some of these claims are so outrageous that scientists do not deem them worthy of investigation whatsoever, but others simply have not been subjected to enough research for the FDA to make reasonable decisions about them.

When to stop research is another open question. If a substance or therapy has shown no effect for a given ailment in hundreds of well-designed trials, does one pursue research into another, slightly different, use? How much time, money, and effort does any modality deserve? The decision to end a line of inquiry is partially dependent on resource prioritization. When the results of research keep coming back negative, other pursuits must eventually take precedence.

Although it may take a long time for a treatment to gain acceptance under the current regulatory regime, the complaints of some practitioners whose methods do not hold up even after being scrutinized are rarely justified. They have careers – and often personal convictions – at stake, and may resort to special pleading to defend their failures. This is exactly what happened in 2005, when the medical journal The Lancet published the most thorough analysis of homeopathy ever conducted, synthesizing data from over 100 trials related to numerous illnesses and found that homeopathy had no better results than placebo. Not only did homeopaths unanimously decry the research; at least one group went as far as to say that placebo-controlled randomized trials were not a proper method for testing their wares:


Two primary concerns for homeopaths are that the treatment is holistic and that it is individualised. Treatment cannot be standardised and patient response is unpredictable. RCTs are looking for specific effects whereas homeopathy is attempting to produce general health effects as well as specific effects – homeopathy treats the whole person….Patients may choose homeopathy or acupuncture or other forms of complementary and alternative medicine because of preferences and perhaps even the potential for responding. Randomized controlled trials do not necessarily recruit such patients – they take patients with a conventional diagnosis, but do not screen for patients who typically may gravitate to homeopathy or other types of complementary and alternative medicine.


In other words, the only proof that these homeopaths can accept is anecdotal – and then, only if it confirms their beliefs. And that is exactly why bodies like the FDA must continue to exist – to divide the purely anecdotal from the thoroughly researched. At its best, that fallible but invaluable service is all we can – or should - expect from it.


References:

1. Carroll, Robert T. “Anecdotal (testimonial) evidence.” The Skepdic’s Dictionary. 1994-2009. 1 Dec. 2009http://www.skeptic.com/testimon.html

2. “Wheatgrass.” Natural Medicines Comprehensive Database. 1995-2009. Therapeutic Research Faculty. 1 Dec. 2009 http://www.naturaldatabase.com/(S(bvtcrabgv0exbxmhw4kbgyar))/nd/Search.aspx?cs=&s=ND&pt=100&id=1073&fs=ND&searchid=18119729

3. Shermer, Michael. “Wheatgrass Juice and Folk Medicine.” Scientific American Magazine. August 2008. 1 Dec. 2009. http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=how-anecdotal-evidence-can-undermine-scientific-results

4. ibid.

5. Baum, Michael. “Should We Maintain an Open Mind about Homeopathy?” The American Journal of Medicine. Volume 122, Issue 11, Pages 973-974. November 2009. Dec. 2, 2009. <http://www.amjmed.com/article/PIIS0002934309005336/fulltext>

6. Alan V. Schmukler. “Interviews: Peter Chappell.” Hpathy e-zine. August 2007. Dec. 1, 2009. http://74.125.93.132/search?q=cache:RHl2oBsaqqwJ:www.hpathy.com/interviews/peterchappell.asp+PC-remedies+aids&cd=6&hl=en&ct=clnk&gl=us

7. “About Us.” Nutrition Business Journal. 2009. Dec. 5, 2009. http://nutritionbusinessjournal.com/aboutus/

8. “Supplements Stand Out As 2008 Sales Bright Spot for U.S. Nutrition Industry.” Nutrition Business Journal. Dec. 4, 2009. Dec. 5, 2009. http://74.125.93.132/search?q=cache:6Z11VcfRn2QJ:subscribers.nutritionbusinessjournal.com/supplements/0601-supplements-bright-spot/+homeopathic+industry+profits+2008&cd=7&hl=en&ct=clnk&gl=us

9. Corte, Corinne. “Why are English Sailors Called Limeys?” Ask a Biologist. Arizona State University. Dec. 1, 2009. http://askabiologist.asu.edu/research/scurvy/index.html

10. “Can Science Make Psychotherapy More Effective?” Science Friday. NPR. Dec. 4, 2009. http://www.npr.org/templates/transcript/transcript.php?storyId=121092295

11. “Complementary and Alternative Medicine – What is it?” Mayo Clinic. Mayo Foundation for Medical Education and Research. Oct. 24, 2009. Dec. 2, 2009. http://www.mayoclinic.com/health/alternative-medicine/PN00001/NSECTIONGROUP=2

12. Noble, Rob. “Alternative, complementary and traditional medicine and HIV.” AVERT. Nov. 12, 2009. Dec. 2, 2009. http://www.avert.org/alternative-medicine-hiv.htm

13. Shang A. et al. “Are the clinical effects of homoeopathy placebo effects? Comparative study of placebo-controlled trials of homoeopathy and allopathy.” Lancet. Aug. 27, 2005. 366(9487). Dec. 2, 2009. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16125589

14. Chatfield K. and Relton C. “Are the clinical effects of homeopathy placebo effects? - A full critique of the article by Shang et al.” European Central Council of Homeopaths. Sep. 2005. Dec. 2, 2009. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16125589

Wednesday, January 6, 2010

Tips on Arguing: Confirmation Bias


Inquiry:

Whether or not you’ve ever lived with someone of the opposite sex, you’re probably familiar with the following scenario:

Maria storms into the living room, where she finds Norm, her husband, watching TV.

“You forgot to put the toilet seat down,” she accuses, her eyes narrowing to narrow slits. “I almost drowned myself.”

Norm rolls his eyes in response. “C’mon,” he says, waving her away with his hand. “So I forgot this once.”

“You always forget!” Maria counters.

“I do not,” he replies. “I remembered every other time this week.”

In the above situation, both Maria and Norm suffer from confirmation bias: the tendency to interpret data based on the parts of it that fit most favorably with our beliefs. Maria sees when Norm leaves the seat up, but she can’t tell when he puts it down. Her memory is filled with her husband’s mistakes. By the same token, Norm recalls all of the times that he thought to put the toilet seat down. But he can’t possibly remember the times when he forgot to put the seat down; if he had, he wouldn’t have left it up. Norm’s memory is filled with examples of his own successes.

According to the Skeptic’s Dictionary, a book and online reference by writer and retired professor of philosophy Robert T. Carroll, Ph.D., confirmation bias is an effect of human psychology. The mind has an easier time building upon preconceived notions than tearing them down. “The tendency to give more attention and weight to the positive and the confirmatory has been shown to influence memory,” he says. “When digging into our memories for data relevant to a position, we are more likely to recall data that confirms the position.”

Although putting the toilet seat down might not be a huge deal, the design and interpretation of scientific experiments is utterly dependent on evading the threat of confirmation bias to the greatest extent possible. One way that researchers handle the problem is by introducing randomized samples into their tests. This keeps them from unwittingly selecting a group of people with a shared characteristic. Another method is by double-blinding a trial; neither the subjects nor the researchers know whether a person belongs to the control group or the experimental group.

Even after an experiment is finished, groups of outside scientists will peer-review the report of what happened. This process helps to ensure that most biases get caught. Not even science, however, can completely escape confirmation bias. That’s why no single study is ever absolute – it must be repeated over and over by others before it can be accepted.Although no one can ever completely eliminate bias, you can mitigate it simply by considering the reasons a person might have for maintaining the opposite viewpoint. It requires more work then you’d think, but the results are worthwhile: your own arguments become more robust, and your mind becomes more open.

Saturday, January 2, 2010

Kudzu

Journey:

In the autumn of 2008, my now-wife and I were renting the second floor of a two-story house. The downstairs had not been rented in months, and in an effort to attract tenants, the landlord had placed several potted flowers on the front stoop during the summer.

As the flowers died, our landlord periodically replaced them with fresh specimens. Some of the older ones, apparently no longer of any aesthetic value, were left to languish in the brush along the side of the house. Most of the plants wilted quickly, but at least one persevered despite the drop in daylight.

On a particularly blustery day, my wife discovered a toppled flower poking out near the bottom of a receding bush, its precious soil spilt and its receptacle cracked. She wouldn’t bear to let it fall prey to neglect, so she collected the remains and brought it upstairs.

When first I saw the thing, it was unrecognizable; a single translucent green stalk, tenuously holding on to three or four leaves and devoid of fruit or flower. As far as we could tell, it might already be dead. We stuck it on the bedside table, in front of a window, with little hope.

The flower took to its new indoor environment with gusto. A week after rescuing it, there were new shoots popping out of the cragged stem. Soon, small flowers ranging from pink to violet with white centers sprouted all over. It appeared that the plant would survive.

We still didn’t know exactly what species the flower was. We were unsure what its life-span might be, how large it could potentially grow, how much light and water it might require. With our other plants, we knew what to expect. We had directions at our disposal to guide our interactions. But in this case, we were forced to learn the flower’s needs and personality through trial and error.

As the plant grew, its demands for water and light increased significantly. If one of us forgot to water it for a single day, the petals would loosen and fall, and the leaves would almost immediately drain of their rigidity. The entire plant would droop rather morbidly. Within an hour or two of watering, it would revert back to its former virility.

By the spring of 2009, the plant had outgrown its pot and our bedside. We gave it a much larger pot of dirt, and moved it in front of the kitchen window.

At times, the plant would inexplicably shed most of its foliage. This event did not seem to follow a particular seasonal pattern. The first time we witnessed it, we suspected that the plant had lived its course. But a few days later that theory was put to rest by the appearance of new buds.

Eventually, the plant got so large that it took over the windowsill. Spices and other small plants that shared the sunlight were choked out by the spindly tentacles of the plant. Some arms had snuck into crevices and snaked in front of the other plants, robbing them of photosynthetic opportunities. Our counter began to resemble the set from Little Shop of Horrors.

We finally could stand no more. The kudzu has been removed from the kitchen and given its own spot in the living room, where most of our monster-sized plants end up. There’s no telling yet whether it will find the new habituĂ© suitable. That may not be such a terrible problem, though – unless it stops expanding, we’re going to need another pot to contain it pretty soon.

And who knows where it will end? Knowing what I now know about the plant’s intentions, I won’t be surprised if I wake up one day to find myself trapped beneath a mesh of leaves and cute pink flowers.


The Creature.